Skip to main content
  • Original research
  • Open access
  • Published:

Performance of Down syndrome subjects during a coincident timing task

Abstract

Background

The time synchronization is a very important ability for the acquisition and performance of motor skills that generate the need to adapt the actions of body segments to external events of the environment that are changing their position in space. Down Syndrome (DS) individuals may present some deficits to perform tasks with synchronization demand. We aimed to investigate the performance of individuals with DS in a simple Coincident Timing task.

Method

32 individuals were divided into 2 groups: the Down syndrome group (DSG) comprised of 16 individuals with average age of 20 (+/− 5 years old), and a control group (CG) comprised of 16 individuals of the same age. All individuals performed the Simple Timing (ST) task and their performance was measured in milliseconds. The study was conducted in a single phase with the execution of 20 consecutive trials for each participant.

Results

There was a significant difference in the intergroup analysis for the accuracy adjustment - Absolute Error (Z = 3.656, p = 0.001); and for the performance consistence - Variable Error (Z = 2.939, p = 0.003).

Conclusion

DS individuals have more difficulty in integrating the motor action to an external stimulus and they also present more inconsistence in performance. Both groups presented the same tendency to delay their motor responses.

Background

Over the years, many studies have shown increasing interest in investigating how the individuals with physical and mental disabilities perform and acquire movements, including those with Down Syndrome (DS) [1]. The main research questions that have been frequently asked in these studies are on: (a) whether such movements can be considered normal or abnormal [2, 3], (b) understanding and explaining the mechanisms and processes underlying the acquisition of these movements [4, 5], and (c) raising subsidies to elaborate new programs for physical practice and therapies [1].

The DS has serious implications in the neural, physiological, and biomechanical systems, which causes the individuals to present some peculiarities, such as is “clumsiness,” that is characterized by movements profile as slow movements with impaired coordination and increased rates of failure, reduced reaction times, decreased muscle tone, impaired control of timing and difficulty to modulate actions under changing task conditions, such as grasp forces and postural responses [612]. Alternative explanations to justify these findings have suggested a different brain activation of the motor response in DS individuals [13].

The atypical patterns of brain organization could be responsible for many cognitive features that generate certain difficulty in organization, perception, and motor response [1]. The movement features of DS individuals can be partially attributed to the structural characteristics, such as the operation of the brain stem and size of cerebellum [13, 14]. The biomechanical profile of DS is an issue that may be attributed as a result of these atypical patterns of brain organization, there is evidence that the individuals with DS present a deficit in performing tasks with predominance in perception requirements, mainly in tasks that demand time synchronization [1518].

According to Schmidt and Wrisberg (2001) [19] the time synchronization is a very important ability for the acquisition and performance of motor skills that generate the need to adapt the actions of body segments to external events of the environment that are changing their position in space. These actions have been given several names over the years, such as: coincident anticipation [19], temporal organization of anticipation [19], timing and anticipation [20, 21], coincident timing [22], and anticipatory timing [23]. In this study, we used the term coincident timing as it is one of the most used in the literature.

In the area of motor learning, the coincident timing has been investigated in healthy subjects during several aspects, for example, the variability of practice [24], stimulus speed [25], age [26], gender [27], level of tasks complexity [28], level of skill [29], knowledge of results [30]. However, when analyzing the literature mentioned above, there are no studies designed for DS individuals involving coincident timing tasks.

It is important to carry out studies that contribute to the comprehension of the motor skill of people with DS [18], especially with respect to the performance of tasks that require coincident timing. Therefore, the aim of this study was to investigate the performance of individuals with DS in a simple Coincident Timing task.

Method

Participants

32 individuals participated in this study and were divided into 2 groups: the Down syndrome group (DSG) comprised of 16 individuals with average age of 20 (+/− 5 years old), classified into slight and moderate, according to the International Classification of Functioning, Disability, and Health (ICF); and a control group (CG) comprised of 16 individuals of the same age. We excluded subjects who were not able to perform the tests. The participation in the experiment was conditioned to the signing of the informed consent form by those responsible. This study was approved by the Independent Ethics Committee of the University with protocol number 942/2010.

Table 1 presents data regarding the severity of impairment.

Table 1 Sample characterization regarding the experimental group

Tasks and apparatus

All individuals performed the Simple Timing (ST) task and their performance was measured in milliseconds. The task was to touch a target in integration with a visual stimulus in such a way that the target would be touched simultaneously with the arrival of a light stimulus at the end of the runway. In order to perform the task, a coincident timing apparatus for complex tasks [31], which is described below, was used.

The apparatus is comprised of a 207 cm long, 10 cm wide, and 2 cm high runway. 96 diodes (LEDs) were placed in a straight line on the runway, 1 cm away from each other. The apparatus is also comprised of a 70 cm long, 40 cm wide, and 6 cm high wooden table on which a sensor was placed. In order to turn on the diodes in sequence, a computer with specific software was used, which was activated by the experimenter.

To perform the simple timing task, the subject sat in front of a table in front of his/her it was disposed an advice with a series of lights arranged in a row that linearly directed toward the participant. When the researcher activated the task, the lights began to illuminate sequentially, always from the farthest to the closest to the subject. The displacement of the light reached a speed of 18 meters per second. The goal of the subject was playing a sensor when the light that was closest to him was synchronously ignited. All subjects performed 20 consecutive attempts, each trial was processed in milliseconds by a specific software unit. From the data extracted by matching software the measurements were calculated. Those measurements present three measurements on the coincidence of the direction (advance or delay) and on the variability of performance, and the absolute error, variable and constant, respectively. The error constant is determined by the average performance of the blocks of trials, the absolute error is determined by the average absolute blocks of trials, and the error variable is determined by the average of the standard deviation of the blocks of each trial [19].

Data regarding basic skills are for sample characterization and items derived from the ICF - International Classification of Functioning and Health. It does not directly influence the performance results but illustrate more clearly the behavior of the sample in these items.

The items assessed were: acquisition of basic competences, acquisition of complex competences, attention and concentration, decision making, performance of complex tasks, and reporting and receiving messages. 1 is the score for slight difficulty and 2 for moderate difficulty.

Data analysis

The results were analyzed in relation to absolute error, variable error, and constant error of each group. These measurements enable the verification of accuracy, consistence, and direction of the performance of each group, respectively.

The Levene's Test was used to assess the homogeneity of variances and it pointed out violation of the homogeneity assumptions for Absolute and Variable errors. Additionally, the Shapiro-Wilk test did not demonstrate the normality assumption in all errors assessed, which precluded the parametric analysis. Thus, in order to verify the performance difference among the groups, the Mann Whitney U test was applied. An alpha of 0.05 for statistical significance was adopted for all analyses.

Results

The Mann Whitney U test pointed out significant difference in the intergroup analysis for the accuracy adjustment of the motor action to the arrival of the visual stimulus at the end of the runway, that is, of the Absolute Error (Z = 3.656, p = 0.001); and for the performance consistence, which reflects in the Variable Error (Z = 2.939, p = 0.003). However, there was no difference among the groups regarding the performance direction, represented by the Constant Error (Z = −1.77, p = 0.07). These results are illustrated in Figure 1.

Figure 1
figure 1

Absolute, constant, and variable error (in milliseconds) obtained through the average of the 20 consecutive attempts performed by each individual. DSG: Down Syndrome Group; CG: control group.

Discussion

The aim of this study was to investigate the performance of individuals with DS in a simple Coincident Timing task, comparing them to healthy individuals. The initial hypothesis of the study was that the DS individuals would present impairment in the timing ability, therefore reflecting in slower and delayed responses.

The results of this experiment confirmed the hypothesis initially raised to the extent the individuals with DS presented a trend of delay in their motor responses, which corroborates the findings in the literature [32, 33]. A possible explanation for these results may be in relation to the neural features presented by these individuals and their reflections in the motor and cognitive aspects, resulting in delay of the motor responses. This inconsistence in performance and inaccuracy when performing motor tasks that involve the time synchronization may be related to the brain stem, as DS individuals, when compared to individuals who do not have the syndrome, present such structure in a smaller size. Such dysfunction associated with the alteration in the cerebellum can justify the slow and delayed responses, as these structures are important for the time synchronization ability.

Based on our study, the absolute error allowed to observe that Down syndrome individuals have a poorer performance compared coincidentally matched individuals. Individuals with DS have greater difficulty performing the coincidence, when compared to controls, which denotes timing problems. According to Kandel (1997) [34] the cerebellum is responsible for coordinating the activity that needs accurate time occurrence, besides coordinating the planning, chronology, and the activity patterns of the skeletal muscles during the movement. Thus, cerebellar dysfunctions may lead to an increase in the time of reaction and abnormalities in the trajectory of the hands, evidencing even more its role of planning and regulation of the movement [35].

Considering the visual-motor system, we reported that the variable error suggests that individuals with Down syndrome tend to perform tasks in unstable and irregular coincidence of visual-motor systems, when compared to control subjects. The main problem of the DS individuals is in the deficit of the perceptual-motor abilities, which are responsible for the abilities that support the acquisition of several motor skills. This deficit can be justified by the structural features mentioned, which explain the difficulties these individuals have to acquire and improve motor skills [18, 19, 32].

Thus, as the coincident timing task depends on selection, planning, and execution processes to anticipate the arrival of the stimulus and perform the effector response simultaneously [22], these abilities are acquired as the perceptual-cognitive mechanism develops [20]. This fact can justify the delayed responses also presented by the control group, as there are studies that evidence greater accuracy in coincident timing tasks with the increase of age [36]. In this context, from 14 or 15 years old the performance curve in this type of task reaches an asymptote, thus making the anticipation ability equivalent to that of an adult [37]. When we observe the age of the individuals of the control group 20 (+/− 5 years old), some of them could be at the final stage of the perceptual-motor development, therefore justifying the delay noticed.

Another aspect to be highlighted is the amount of practice offered in the design of this study, which could explain the delay in responses, as the number of attempts was not enough to improve performance. Freudenheim (1994) [38] states that it is necessary to investigate whether the performance in a simple coincident timing task in adults is proportional to the number of attempts. It was observed that 90 attempts in a task of the same type were needed to ensure an optimum level of performance [39]. In this case, the delayed motor responses of the control group may be associated with the number of attempts offered. However, it should be noted that it is not a motor learning study, but an investigation of the performance in this type of task in a population with Down syndrome.

Nevertheless, as an important limitation of your study, the use of a mental age-matched control group in line with age is fundamental, as according to Sugden and Keogh (1990) [14] the DS individuals present a context of delay since the early phases of life and there is a tendency that such deficit increases over the years due to the reduction of experiences with time synchronization tasks, when compared to individuals who do not have the syndrome. The IQ of adult subjects with DS is approximately 40 to 70 points, while healthy adults are near 100 [40]. This difference should be accounted for when studying cognitive functions in DS.

A question to be answered refers to the impact of the alteration on the ability to perform timing tasks when performing daily activities. In this context, Gimenez, Stefanoni and Farias (2007) [18] state that individuals with DS can have a deficit in the motor ability regarding time synchronization and still be able to perform movement patterns that depend on such ability in a proper way or in a way that satisfactorily meets the environmental requirements throughout life, not reflecting in the deterioration of the performance of daily activities. However, this fact still needs future research.

It is known that, in manual tasks, the individuals with DS present characteristics such as slowness, selection of unusual strategies, and delay in the acquisition of key patterns of movement [17, 33]. Another explanatory hypothesis for the slowness in movement is the fact that these individuals perform the movement slower in order to achieve more accuracy. This difference is due to the strategy adopted for motor control and therefore it is considered an adaptive reaction, and not necessarily a biological difference.

An important point in our study is important to be raised. Although we used a task that involves specific cognitive abilities, our main purpose was not to study the influence of cognitive status on the specific mentioned tests. We suggest a further experiment to specifically investigate this issue.

Our study presents important findings, since motor development has been recently investigated [4144]. The task performed in this experiment has been criticized due to the fact that it is very simple for individuals that do not have the Down syndrome, which prevents the results achieved from being generalized for the performance of activities in the real world [26]. However, we believe it is a relevant task for individuals with DS, as time synchronization tasks are present in the daily routine of these individuals, such as crossing the street, entering the elevator, passing in the turnstile, climbing the escalator or even taking an object thrown by another person. In this sense, future studies can approach more complex tasks and with more ecologic validity, comparing them to more controlled tasks performed in laboratories, investigating the impact of the impairment on the time synchronization ability when performing daily activities.

Conclusion

Individuals with DS, when compared to individuals of the same age who do not have the Syndrome, have more difficulty in integrating the motor action to an external stimulus and they also present more inconsistence in performance. However, even though the DS group had a higher level of delay, both groups presented the same tendency to delay their motor responses.

References

  1. Groen MA, Yasin I, Laws G, Barry JG, Bishop DV: Weak hand preference in children with Down syndrome is associated with language deficits. Develop Psychobiology 2008, 50:242–250.

    Article  CAS  Google Scholar 

  2. Latash ML: Learning motor synergies by persons with Down syndrome. J Intellectual Disabil Res 2007, 51:962–971.

    Article  CAS  Google Scholar 

  3. Mulvey GM, Ringenbach SD, Jung ML: Reversal of handedness effects on bimanual coordination in adults with Down syndrome. J Intellectual Disabil Res 2011, 55:998–1007.

    Article  CAS  Google Scholar 

  4. González-Agüero A, Vicente-Rodríguez G, Gómez-Cabello A, Ara I, Moreno LA, Casajús JA: A combined training intervention program increases lean mass in youths with Down syndrome. Res Developm Disabil 2011, 32:2383–2388.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  5. Mendonça GV, Pereira FD, Fernhall B: Effects of combined aerobic and resistance exercise training in adults with and without Down syndrome. Arch Phys Med Rehabil 2011, 92:37–45.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  6. Lana-Elola E, Watson-Scales SD, Fisher EM, Tybulewicz VL: Down syndrome: searching for the genetic culprits. Disease Mod Mechan 2011, 4:586–595.

    Article  CAS  Google Scholar 

  7. Lam MY, Hodges NJ, Virji-Babul N, Latash ML: Evidence for slowing as a function of index of difficulty in young adults with Down syndrome. American J Intel Developm Disabil 2009, 114:411–426.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  8. Kearney K, Gentile AM: Prehension in young children with Down syndrome. Acta Psycholog 2002, 112:3–16.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  9. Rihtman T, Tekuzener E, Parush S, Tenenbaum A, Bachrach SJ, Ornoy A: Are the cognitive functions of children with Down syndrome related to their participation? Develop Med Child Neurol 2010, 52:72–78.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  10. Charlton JL, Ihsen E, Oxley J: The influence of context in the development of reaching and grasping: Implications for assessment disability. In Motor behavior and human skill: a multidisciplinary approach. Edited by: Piek JP. Champaign, IL: Human Kinetics; 1998:283–302.

    Google Scholar 

  11. Davis WR, Kelso JAS: Analysis of “invariant characteristics” in the motor control of Down’s syndrome and normal subjects. J Motor Beh 1982, 14:194–212.

    CAS  Google Scholar 

  12. Henderson SE, Morris J, Frith V: The motor deficit in Down’s syndrome children: A problem of timing? J Child Psycholog Psych 1981, 22:233–245.

    Article  CAS  Google Scholar 

  13. Millan Sanchez M, Heyn SN, Das D, Moghadam S, Martin KJ, Salehi A: Neurobiological elements of cognitive dysfunction in Down syndrome: exploring the role of APP. Biolog Psych 2011, 24:101–111.

    Google Scholar 

  14. Sugden DA, Keogh JF: Problems in skill development. South Caroline: University of South Caroline Press; 1990.

    Google Scholar 

  15. Galli M, Rigoldi C, Brunner R, Virji-Babul N, Giorgio A: Joint stiffness and gait pattern evaluation in children with Down syndrome. Gait Posture 2008, 28:502–506.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  16. Latash ML: Learning motor synergies by persons with Down syndrome. J Intellect Disabil Res 2007, 51:962–971.

    Article  PubMed  CAS  Google Scholar 

  17. Block ME: Motor development in children with Down syndrome: a review of the literature. Adapted Phys Activ Quart 1991, 8:179–209.

    Google Scholar 

  18. Gimenez R, Stefanoni FF, Farias PB: Relation between temporal synchronization ability and fundamental movement patterns striking and catching in individuals with and without Down syndrome. Rev Bras Ciência Mov 2007, 15:95–101.

    Google Scholar 

  19. Schmidt RA: Anticipation and timing in human motor performance. Psycholog Bull 1968, 70:631–646.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  20. Dorfman PW: Timing and anticipation: a developmental perspective. J Motor Beh 1977, 9:67–79.

    Google Scholar 

  21. Schmidt R, Wrisberg CA: Aprendizagem e performance motora: uma abordagem de aprendizagem baseada no problema. Porto Alegre: Artmed; 2001.

    Google Scholar 

  22. Beslise JJ: Accuracy, reliability and refractoriness in a coincidence-anticipation task. Res Quart 1963, 34:271–281.

    Google Scholar 

  23. Magill RA: Motor learning: concepts and application. 5a edn. Dubuque: WCB/Mcgraw-Hill; 1998.

    Google Scholar 

  24. Freudenheim AM, Tani G: Efeitos da estrutura de prática variada na aprendizagem de uma tarefa de “ timing ” coincidente em crianças. Rev Paul Educ Fís 1995, 9:87–98.

    Google Scholar 

  25. Teixeira LA, Santos VA, Andreysuk R: Tarefas que envolvem “ timing ” antecipatório: seriam as velocidades mais baixas as mais fáceis para sincronizar? Rev Paul Educ Fís 1992, 6:21–28.

    Google Scholar 

  26. Ferraz OL: Desenvolvimento de timing antecipatório em crianças. São Paulo: Escola de Educação Física e Esporte, Universidade de São Paulo; 1993.

    Google Scholar 

  27. Dunham Junior P, Reeve J: Sex, eye experience and speed of stimulus effect on anticipation of coincidence. Percept Motor Skills 1990, 45:1171–1176.

    Google Scholar 

  28. Williams LRT, Jasiewicz J, Simmons RW: Coincidence timing of finger, arm, and whole body movements. Percept Motor Skills 2001, 92:535–547.

    Article  PubMed  CAS  Google Scholar 

  29. Ripoll H, Latiri I: Effect of expertise on coincident- timing accuracy in a fast ball game. J Sports Sciences 1997, 15:573–580.

    Article  CAS  Google Scholar 

  30. Magill RA, Chamberlin CJ, Hall KG: Verbal knowledge of results as redundant information for learning an anticipation timing skill. Hum Mov Science 1991, 10:485–502.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  31. Corrêa UC, Tani G: Aparelho de timing coincidente em tarefas complexas. São Paulo: Revista da Propriedade Industrial; 178. [P.I. nº 0.403.433–4 de 03/08/2004] RPI n.1763, 19/10/2004

  32. Schwartzman JS: Síndrome de Down. São Paulo: Memon; 1997.

    Google Scholar 

  33. Henderson SE: Motor skill development. In Current approaches to Down syndrome. Edited by: Lane D, Stratford B. London: Cassell; 1985:187–218.

    Google Scholar 

  34. Kandel ER, Schwartz JH, Jessel TM: Fundamentos da neurociência e do comportamento. Rio de Janeiro: Guanabara-Koogan; 1997.

    Google Scholar 

  35. Kandel ER, Schwartz JH, Jessel TM: Princípios da neurociência, 4a edn. São Paulo: Manole; 2003.

    Google Scholar 

  36. Bard C, Fleury M, Gagnon M: Coincidence-anticipation timing : an age related perspective. In Development of eye-hand coordination across life span. Edited by: Bard C, Fleury M, Hay L. Columbia: University of South Carolina; 1990:283–305.

    Google Scholar 

  37. Fleury M, Bard C: Age, stimulus velocity and task complexity as determiners of coincident timing behavior. J Hum Mov Studies 1985, 11:305–317.

    Google Scholar 

  38. Stadulis RE: Coincidence-anticipation behavior of children. In Motor development: current selected research. Edited by: Clark JE, Humphrey JH. Princeton: Princeton Book; 1985.

    Google Scholar 

  39. Freudenheim AM: Um teste à formação de esquema: efeito da variabilidade e da quantidade de prática na produção de movimentos novos em adultos. Rev Paul Educ Fís 1994, 8:3–16.

    Google Scholar 

  40. Mégarbané A, Noguier F, Stora S, Manchon L, Mircher C, Bruno R, Dorison N, Pierrat F, Rethoré MO, Trentin B, Ravel A, Morent M, Lefranc G, Piquemal D: The intellectual disability of trisomy 21: differences in gene expression in a case series of patients with lower and higher IQ. Eur J Hum Genet 2013. Feb 20. [Epub ahead of print]

    Google Scholar 

  41. Silva F, Arias-Carrión O, Teixeira S, Velasques B, Peressutti C, Paes F, Basile LF, Menéndez-González M, Murillo-Rodríguez E, Cagy M, Piedade R, Nardi AE, Machado S, Ribeiro P: Functional coupling of sensorimotor and associative areas during a catching ball task: a qEEG coherence study. Int Arch Med 2012, 5:9.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  42. Coppede AC, Okuda PMM, Capellini AS: Performance of children with learning difficulties in fine motor function and handwriting. J Human Growth Develop 2012, 22:297–306.

    Google Scholar 

  43. de Morais FD, Freitas JC, Viana FP, Formiga CKMR: Correlation between neurofunctional profile and sensory-motor skills of children with cerebral palsy. J Human Growth Develop 2012, 22:226–232.

    Google Scholar 

  44. Oliveira DKS, Do Nascimento DDG, Marcolino FF: Perceptions of family caregivers and professionals in the family health strategy related to the care and neuropsychomotor development of children. J Human Growth Develop 2012, 22:142–150.

    Google Scholar 

Download references

Acknowledgements

This study received financial support from School of Physical Education and Sport, University of Sao Paulo. We would like to thank ARIL - Limeirense Infant Rehabilitation Association for all the assistantship during data collection.

Author information

Authors and Affiliations

Authors

Corresponding author

Correspondence to Camila Torriani-Pasin.

Additional information

Competing interests

We declare no conflict of interest.

Authors’ contributions

CTP, GMGB, MAAS, GLA, GCSP, CBMM, LCA, VEV, APJ, RWW and UCC participated in the acquisition of data and revision of the manuscript. All authors conceived of the study, determined the design, interpreted the data and drafted the manuscript. CTP, VEV, LCA and UCC determined the design and drafted the manuscript. All authors read and gave final approval for the version submitted for publication.

Rights and permissions

Reprints and permissions

About this article

Cite this article

Torriani-Pasin, C., Bonuzzi, G.M., Soares, M.A. et al. Performance of Down syndrome subjects during a coincident timing task. Int Arch Med 6, 15 (2013). https://doi.org/10.1186/1755-7682-6-15

Download citation

  • Received:

  • Accepted:

  • Published:

  • DOI: https://doi.org/10.1186/1755-7682-6-15

Keywords